• Open Access
    Original Article

    Screening tools for dementia assessment in UK based ethnic minorities

    Clara Calia 1*
    Mario A. Parra 2

    Explor Med. 2024;5:401–415 DOI: https://doi.org/10.37349/emed.2024.00227

    Received: January 25, 2024 Accepted: April 20, 2024 Published: June 06, 2024

    Academic Editor: Hua Su, University of California, USA

    This article belongs to the special issue Neurophysiological Mechanisms of Aging and Dementia

    Abstract

    Aim:

    The present study investigated whether commonly used screening tools and assessments for dementia were culturally appropriate for older adults from ethnic minorities (EM) groups living in the UK.

    Methods:

    Both South Asian and British participants (N = 43) were assessed using the Cross-Linguistic Naming Test, Mini Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination, Visual Short-Term Memory Binding Test (VSTMBT), and the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale. Multi-Ethnic Acculturation Scale and English proficiency, measured with a self-rated scale, were associated with the four respective. No interpreters were used.

    Results:

    While members from EM significantly differed from members of the ethnic majority group in traditional neuropsychological tasks, their performance on the VSTMBT yielded results comparable to those drawn from the ethnic majority group. Complex influences seem to drive the sensitivity of traditional neuropsychological tasks to sociocultural factors.

    Conclusions:

    This is the first study that subjects the VSTMBT to investigation in EM groups. Older adults from EM showed no impact of their sociocultural backgrounds on the function assessed by this test. However, other tests widely used for the assessment of EM populations proved sensitive to the investigated sociocultural factors. Our results lend support to the suggestion that neuropsychological assessments must abandon the one-size-fits-all notion when it comes to dementia risk detection among EM groups.

    Keywords

    Cross-cultural neuropsychology, ethnic minorities, dementia, interpreters, screening tools, UK

    Introduction

    There are around 25,000 dementia patients with ethnic minority (EM) backgrounds in the United Kingdom, and this figure is anticipated to double by 2025 [14]. As a result, neuropsychologists will inevitably interact with individuals from diverse cultural, linguistic and educational backgrounds throughout their clinical practices [5]. Such a context poses challenges for health professionals and policymakers attempting to guarantee equity in health care [6]. Members of EMs are often diagnosed later than the rest of the population [7, 8] due to late (self)referrals and lack of appropriate tests and staff training [4, 911]. Therefore, it is increasingly important to develop instruments to screen for dementia in people from EM groups [12]. Although symptoms of dementia do not differ between different races and ethnicities, cross-cultural assessment of dementia can often be challenging for several reasons including: 1) lack of culturally valid tools for assessment, 2) language barrier, 3) culturally embedded stigma and taboo, 4) impact of stereotype threat (assumption by the examiner/cultural majority that people from a different cultural background will perform more poorly than people from a cultural majority background) and 5) prejudice on the part of the clinician or patient [11, 13]. One problem is the potential for misdiagnosis (false positive/negative) of cognitive decline amongst EM. The cognitive tests that are often used for cognitive screening are in many cases not suitable for people with a minority background (e.g., MMSE [14]). Linguistic, cultural, and educational factors have been shown to significantly influence cognitive test results [1518]. Most available tests were developed in Western countries and have not gone through proper validation for their use in cross-cultural settings. They are therefore prone to cultural, educational, and linguistic biases [19, 20], rendering their use with EM groups inappropriate. Neuropsychological tests designed and standardized to measure constructs in one culture may not be readily applied to individuals of other cultures, with an expectation that they will equally measure the same construct [2123]. This problem is exacerbated by a large gap in the literature for validation of neuropsychological tests for EMs in the UK [24]. There is a growing need of brief culture-free screening tool for dementia [2530].

    As briefly mentioned above, a strategy to overcome cultural bias is the development of theory-driven cross-cultural neuropsychological tests [31, 32] that possess properties of dementia markers [33]. Such tests would avoid the use of culture-specific or verbal stimuli [34]. Culture-free or unbiased tests could be used in the same format in different cultural contexts, removing the need for lengthy and laborious adaptations that often undermine their construct validity. By minimising linguistic elements, they have the potential to be suitable for individuals from EMs being assessed in a language other than their own [12, 35]. An example is the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale (RUDAS [36, 37]), a screening test developed as an alternative to the MMSE to minimize the effects of culture, language, and education on assessment outcomes [38].

    An important challenge cross-cultural neuropsychology faces in global societies is linked to health service providers [10, 11]. Fujii [10] highlighted the challenge that assigning non-native English speakers with the assistance of translators poses to neuropsychologists. When investigating cognitive function in individuals with a minority background, where language constitutes a barrier, it is recommended to use a professionally trained interpreter who speaks the person’s mother tongue [13, 39]. Healthcare professionals often lack training in working with interpreters which could lead to poor communication, and, in turn, affect the quality of clinical evaluations [40, 41]. Using non-professional interpreters can result in inaccuracy in assessment outcomes, loss of confidentiality, and conflicts of interest [42]. A skilled interpreter can help prevent misdiagnosis when tests are administered in a non-native language [43, 44]. However, brief cognitive screening tools are often administered by practitioners in primary care settings where interpreters may be unavailable [45, 46]. Worldwide there are around 6,800 different languages, interpreters for each language are simply unrealistic [47]. Furthermore, using interpreters does not reduce the impact of culturally inappropriate content on test performance [48]. Taken together, the paucity of professional interpreters available in health settings across the world [11, 13], the risk of literal translation masking clinically relevant details [49], and the possibility of translator biases, i.e., answer on the patients’ behalf [50], render the use of interpreters an unreliable source.

    The present study

    The current study aimed to investigate whether four cognitive tests used to screen for dementia, namely the Mini Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (M-ACE [51]), Cross-Linguistic Naming Test (CLNT [52]), the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale (RUDAS [34]), the Visual Short-Term Memory Binding Test (VSTMBT [53, 54]), reveal any bias in performance associated to participants’ ethnicity. Additionally, the effects of education, acculturation and language proficiency, as potential drivers of performance bias, were also investigated [55], along with self-rated English proficiency scores. A key methodological characteristic of the study is that we did not use interpreters. We predicted that, since a limited language involvement is required for the VSTMBT, performance on this test would be insensitive to the ethnic background of those assessed, a pattern that would not hold for the other screening tests.

    Materials and methods

    Participants

    To be eligible, participants had to be aged 60 or over, resident in the UK and needed to meet the following group-specific criteria:

    1. EM group: a) identify as belonging to a South Asian EM group, b) speak English as an additional (non-native) language, c) have sufficient mastery of English to understand test instructions and give informed consent. 2. Ethnic majority group: a) identify as belonging to the UK ethnic majority group, b) speak English as a first language. Participants were excluded if they met any of the following criteria: Diagnosis of dementia or any other neurological or health conditions (e.g., stroke, psychiatric illness) that may affect cognitive functioning; disabilities that could interfere with testing or would require special accommodations (e.g., uncorrected vision or hearing problems). No subjects who met the inclusion criteria were excluded due to the above criteria.

    To determine whether participants included in each group were cognitively healthy, authors employed a combination of self-report and established inclusion/exclusion criteria. Self-report: Participants were asked to self-report their medical history, current health status, and any cognitive concerns at the start of the assessment. They were asked questions related to memory, attention, language abilities, and overall cognitive functioning. Self-report allows researchers to gather initial information about participants’ cognitive health directly from the individuals themselves.

    The final sample consisted of 43 subjects. The EM group comprises South Asian participants (N = 23; but see footnote of Table 1 for missing data), mainly from India and Pakistan, as they represent the largest EM in Scotland at 2.7% of the total population [56] and 2.3% in the UK [57]. Ethnic majority participants (N = 20) were recruited through a combination of convenience, snowball, and purposive sampling.

    Assessments

    CLNT

    CLNT was used to determine language proficiency. The CLNT is a short test that assesses a person’s ability to identify and name 40 English words included within six semantic categories: animals, actions, parts of the body, natural phenomena, external objects, and colours [52]. Items are based on the Swadesh word list, used in nearly all of the extant languages spoken today [52, 58, 59]. The 100 vocabulary words within the Swadesh list are designed to be entirely disparate within language families irrespective of their cultural, geographic, or environmental proximity [58, 59]. The test incorporates words within the Swadesh list to avoid the potential confound of tester unfamiliarity with a particular word due to a potential difference in linguistic background. For this reason, the Swadesh words are incorporated as they are considered to be more frequently used in nearly all of the extant languages spoken today [52]. Studies have also shown the CLNT to be culturally and linguistically sensitive amongst differing cultural groups from Columbia, Morocco, Spain [60] and Lebanon [61].

    M-ACE

    The M-ACE is the abbreviated version of the ACE-III [51], a screening test for dementia. The ACE-III is a widely adopted and validated test currently used in memory clinics and dementia research around the world. Cultural adaptations for various items have been made to the ACE-III when it has been translated into languages other than English [62]. Since the M-ACE places heavy emphasis on verbally mediated and language items [51], it is likely that the EM group using English as an additional language will underperform compared to the ethnic and linguistic majority group. M-ACE has also been shown to be culturally valid in Malaysian [63], Thai [64], and Chinese [65] populations. However, each of these studies examined translated versions of the M-ACE. To date, no study has indicated whether the English M-ACE is culturally appropriate and sensitive to immigrants and EM living within English speaking countries.

    RUDAS

    The RUDAS is a brief cognitive screening instrument for dementia. It was developed for ethnically diverse populations [36] and is less influenced by educational and cultural factors than the MMSE [20, 37, 66, 67]. The RUDAS has been translated into over 30 languages [68] with strong ecological validity [20], and it has been shown to have comparable diagnostic accuracy for dementia to other conventional dementia screening tools, such as the MMSE and the ACE-III [37, 66, 69].

    It comprises of 6 subtests [36] with limited emphasis on linguistic abilities. Moreover, there is limited evidence on the acceptability of the RUDAS for non-native English speakers without support from interpreters. The RUDAS was originally developed for use with an interpreter if the clinician does not speak the first language of the patient [36]. In a systematic review of the RUDAS, all eleven studies used formal interpreters [20]. It is critical to determine whether the RUDAS is appropriate in situations where interpreters are unavailable. In a study by Gonçalves et al. [70], performance of participants with English as a first and second language was compared on the RUDAS, without an interpreter. Both groups performed similarly, indicating that the RUDAS can be administered in a participant’s second language. However, level of language proficiency was not measured, so all participants may have been highly proficient in English. Furthermore, while the validity of it has been established in a range of Western European countries [71] it is yet to be administered with a UK-based sample.

    VSTMBT

    The VSTMBT is a theory-driven domain-specific cognitive assessment. It assesses the precise function of temporarily binding the features of complex objects (i.e., shape and colour) together. This function is different (i.e., dissociable) from the one needed to process either feature separately. Contrary to process-impure cognitive abilities or composite scores, the latter known to encapsulate several outcome measures without underpinning psychometric frameworks [72, 73], theory-driven domain-specific cognitive assessments stand a better chance of both detecting the disease earlier and with better specificity and advancing theories of memory decline in ageing and dementia [74]. Such psychometric properties explain why VSTMB is insensitive to normal ageing [i.e., independent of the hippocampus [7577], which shrinks with age, and reliant on regions of the medial temporal lobe (MTL) known to remain intact across the lifespan—perirhinal/entorhinal cortex [78]]. However, such MTL regions (anterior MTL network) are known to be affected by Alzheimer’s disease much earlier than the hippocampus [79, 80]. This explains why the VSTMBT detects Alzheimer’s disease preclinically in its familial [54] and sporadic variants [27] even when other traditional neuropsychological tests fail to detect impairments. Logie et al. [33] suggested that a good marker for Alzheimer’s disease should be insensitive to normal ageing, sensitive and specific to Alzheimer’s disease, and insensitive to the education level, literacy and cultural background of those assessed. We have now contributed consistent evidence supporting the former two. This study aims to contribute new evidence suggesting that this new assessment also meets the third criterion above.

    The VSTMBT has been considered a cross-cultural marker for Alzheimer’s disease [32]. During the VSTMBT, participants were presented with arrays of two or three abstract shapes displayed in random positions of a “3 × 3” virtual grid. After an initial fixation cross (1,000 ms), a study display was presented for 2,000 ms followed by a 900 ms unfilled retention interval. The test display was then presented and remained on until participants responded. Participants were asked to detect whether a change occurred between the study and the test display or if the stimuli remained the same. The task consisted of two conditions, the Single Shape condition assessed VSTMBT for single features. The Binding condition assessed VSTMBT for shape-colour bindings. During the Single Shape condition, participants were presented with either two or three black shapes, and in the Binding condition, two or three shapes were presented each in a different colour. To be able to detect such changes, participants had to remember either the individual shapes or the combinations of shape and colour (i.e., Binding condition) presented in the study display. Each condition consisted of 32 trials, of which 50% presented arrays of two stimuli and the remaining 50% presented three stimuli. The rationale for the use of these two set sizes was presented by Parra et al. (2019) [81]. We recorded the percentage of correct recognition per task condition and set size. The VSTMBT has been considered a cross-cultural marker for Alzheimer’s disease [32]. The function assessed by this test (i.e., binding in short-term memory), has proved insensitive to participants’ education and cultural background [82, 83].

    Additional questionnaires

    Education—RQF is a score between 1–8 based on the UK’s Regulated Qualifications Framework (2019). Education Level was calculated by dividing three levels RQF [level 1 ≤ GCSE (General Certificate of Secondary Education); level 2 > GCSE; level 3 is a bachelor’s degree or higher]. Years in the UK, the percentage years in the UK and English proficiency were retrieved from the Multi-Ethnic Acculturation Scale (MAS [55]). English proficiency was recorded by a self-reported scale between excellent and poor from the MAS [55], the only acculturation scale designed for use with individuals from diverse ethnic groups [84]. This scale uses simple, commonly used vocabulary, suitable for linguistic minorities.

    Data analysis

    Data from neuropsychological tests were compared across groups using either independent-samples t-tests or Mann-Whitney U-tests (see Table S1 for normality checks and Table S2 for non-parametric tests). We reported on the outcomes from parametric tests here and those of non-parametric analysis are shown in Supplementary material. This methodological decision aims to demonstrate that the significance of the results observed was independent of the type of statistical analysis used. Variables from the VSTMBT were compared using a parametric 3-way mixed ANOVA model with Group (EM vs. ethnic majority) as the between-subjects factors, and Condition (shape vs. binding) and Set Size (2 vs. 3) as the within-subjects factors. A Group × Condition interaction will suggest potential cultural bias and this would be followed up with appropriate tests. If proved valid, we calculated the Binding Cost [7] which is the variable considered a marker for Alzheimer’s disease [27, 53, 54]. Finally, stepwise regression models were developed to explore underpinnings of the sensitivity of the screening tests investigated to sociocultural factors. The data were analysed using IBM SPSS Statistics (version 28).

    Procedures

    The data collected in this manuscript were obtained in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study received ethical approval by the University of Edinburgh’s School of Health in Social Science (CLIN623). Every participant provided written consent. The average assessment time was approximately forty minutes.

    Results

    There were some missing data in the final dataset [no significant differences in age, education, or gender were identified between groups (Table 1)]. However, statistical differences for years in the UK [t (34.99) = –7.60, P < 0.001], and percentages of years in the UK [t (28.95) = –8.84, P < 0.001], and English proficiency [t (40.24) = –6.50, P < 0.001] and MAS UK [t (41.00) = –5.21, P < 0.001] confirmed the appropriateness of the sample recruited to test the hypotheses here investigated.

    Three-way mixed ANOVA model for the VSTMBT

    There was a significant effect of Group [F (1,40) = 6.12, P = 0.018, η2 = 0.133]. Both the effect of Condition [F (1,40) = 66.89, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.63] and Set Size [F (1,40) = 33.31, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.45] were significant. Interestingly, none of the interactions reached the significance threshold [Group × Condition: F (1,40) = 2.43, P = 0.127, η2 = 0.06; Group × Set Size: F (1,40) = 0.41, P = 0.524, η2 = 0.01; Set Size × Condition: F (1,40) = 0.06, P = 0.805, η2 = 0.002; Group × Condition × Set Size: F (1,40) = 0.62, P = 0.805, η2 = 0.002] (Figure 1). We, therefore, collapsed performance across Set Size, and used such scores to calculate the Binding Cost [Binding Cost = (Performance on Shape-Only – Performance on Shape-Colour Binding)/Performance on Shape-Only * 100, [85]] which entered further analyses.

    Mean data that entered the mixed ANOVA model

    Among the neuropsychological tests, the CLNT [t (20.27) = –2.19, P = 0.04], M-ACE [t (29.52) = –3.42, P < 0.001] and RUDAS [t (27.82) = –2.16, P = 0.04] revealed significant differences across Group, while the VSTMBT, particularly the cost of binding [t (40) = 1.16, P = 0.27], did not (Table 1).

    Demographic and cognitive characteristic

    Dependent VariablesEthnic Minorities GroupEthnic MinoritiesStatistic
    NMean (SD)NMean (SD)tdfP-valueCI 95%
    Age2067.55 (6.19)2266.59 (8.71)–0.4140.000.69(–5.71 3.80)
    Education RQF208.05 (13.26)2216.79 (20.89)1.6335.940.11(–2.12 19.59)
    Education level202.30 (0.57)222.50 (0.80)0.9240.000.36(–0.24 0.64)
    Years in the UK2065.70 (7.71)2339.84 (14.07)–7.6034.99< 0.001(–32.77 –18.95)
    Percentage years in the UK2097.37 (7.94)2258.88 (18.63)–8.8428.95< 0.001(–47.38 –29.58)
    English proficiency204.75 (0.55)233.48 (0.73)–6.5040.24< 0.001(–1.67 –0.88)
    Multi-ethnic Acculturation Scale2085.05 (12.10)2364.61 (13.43)–5.2141.00< 0.001(–28.36 –12.52)
    Neuropsychological tests
    Cross-Linguistic Naming Test2039.90 (0.31)2138.05 (3.87)–2.1920.270.04(–3.62 –0.09)
    M-ACE2027.15 (2.94)2321.39 (7.44)–3.4229.52< 0.001*(–9.20 –2.32)
    RUDAS2028.30 (2.64)2224.95 (6.73)–2.1627.820.04*(–6.52 –0.17)
    VSTMBT Perception209.90 (0.31)229.64 (1.14)–1.0524.350.31*(–0.78 0.26)
    VSTMBT 2 Shapes2015.10 (1.83)2214.18 (2.22)–1.4540.000.15*(–2.19 0.36)
    VSTMBT 2 Bindings2013.20 (2.19)2211.27 (2.55)–2.6240.000.01*(–3.42 –0.44)
    VSTMBT Cost 22012.54 (10.12)2219.72 (16.39)1.7340.000.09*(–1.25 15.63)
    VSTMBT 3 Shapes2013.65 (2.43)2212.91 (2.52)–0.9740.000.34*(–2.29 0.81)
    VSTMBT 3 Bindings2011.75 (1.89)2210.23 (1.54)–2.8740.000.01*(–2.59 –0.45)
    VSTMBT Cost 3209.67 (30.68)2218.4 (16.59)1.1640.000.27*(–6.45 23.90)
    Display full size

    M-ACE: Mini Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination; RQF: Regulated Qualifications Framework; RUDAS: Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale; VSTMBT: Visual Short-Term Memory Binding Test. * not all the neuropsychological variables were normally distributed, see Tables S1 and S2, for non-parametric Mann-Whitney U tests. There were a few missing data from the Ethnic Minorities Group: one participant did not report on age or percentage years in the UK, and one did not report on education. One did not contribute data on the Cross-Linguistic Naming Test or RUDAS, and one did not contribute data on the Cross-Linguistic Naming Test or the VSTMBT

    Regression models

    To explore the potential underpinnings of the sociocultural sensitivity shown by the neuropsychological screening tests, we ran regression models (Table 2). For the stepwise regression analysis, Group was first included as the predictor of interest (Model 1). A subsequent model included education level, years in the UK, percentage years in the UK, English proficiency, and the MAS as additional regressors (Model 2). Such an approach allowed assessment of whether variability on the cognitive screening tests could be accounted for by the participants’ ethnic background before (Model 1), and after (Model 2) controlling for sociocultural factors.

    Regression model

    Dependent VariablesModel 1Model 2
    R2ANOVAAdj-R2ANOVA
    CLNT10.5%F (1,39) = 4.56, P = 0.03954.2%F (3,35) = 15.98, P < 0.001 (a, d, f)
    M-ACE20.4%F (1,41) = 10.52, P = 0.00222.2%F (1,39) = 12.39, P = 0.001 (f)
    RUDAS9.8%F (1,40) = 4.33, P = 0.04431.8%F (2,37) = 10.08, P < 0.001 (c and d)
    VSTMB Cost Total6.3%F (1,40) = 3.76, P = 0.059No variables entered a model
    Display full size

    ACE: Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination; CLNT: Cross-Linguistic Naming Test; RUDAS: Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale; VSTMB Cost Total: Average of Visual Short-Term Memory Binding Cost 2 and 3. (a) Group, (b) education level, (c) years in the UK, (d) percentage years in the UK, (e) English proficiency, and the (f) MAS as additional regressors

    Ethnicity was a significant predictor of CLNT. This predictive value remained significant after controlling for other sociocultural factors, with percentage of years in the UK and scores on the MAS also retained as significant predictors. Ethnicity significantly predicted performance on the M-ACE and RUDAS. However, after controlling for other sociocultural factors, ethnicity was no longer a significant predictor. The MAS was retained as a predictor of performance on the M-ACE whereas education level, years in the UK, and percentage of years in the UK were retained as predictors of performance on the RUDAS. Neither ethnicity nor other sociocultural factors yielded models that significantly predicted the cost of binding (see Table 2).

    Discussion

    The presented study aimed to investigate whether cognitive tests used to screen for dementia revealed bias in test performance based on an individual’s ethnicity. The key points are: (1) Traditional neuropsychological tasks (CLNT, M-ACE and RUDAS) have proved sensitive to sociocultural variables whereas; (2) the VSTMBT proved insensitive. (3) Complex influences seem to drive the sensitivity of traditional neuropsychological tasks to sociocultural as demonstrated by regression models. We proceed to discuss these findings in turn.

    Traditional neuropsychological tests

    Performance on the CLNT seems to be influenced by the percentage of years in the UK. The words found within the CLNT are derived from the Swadesh word list [58], a set of words considered to be universally found in most of the spoken languages. However, major criticisms have arisen about the degree to which the Swadesh words are universal [86]. The principal critique is that the Swadesh word list presumes that there are direct word equivalents between English and the many incorporated languages in the list. For example, the English word “you”, found in the Swadesh vocabulary, does not have a direct one-to-one word equivalent in other languages such as Urdu, a primarily South Asian language spoken by many throughout India and Pakistan [87].

    Level of acculturation was a significant predictor of the M-ACE. The significant moderate correlation between acculturation and M-ACE suggests that higher adoption of the dominant culture is related to better test performance above age and education [84]. As culture represents adaptation to live in a specific context [88], what is relevant and worth learning for one individual in a particular cultural context may be less relevant for someone from another cultural background [89].

    The M-ACE has been validated in different translated language versions [6365], but it was not designed as a cross-cultural dementia assessment [51]. Thus, the results for the M-ACE test between the two cultural groups are not particularly surprising. These findings indicate that using the M-ACE in an EM group when administered in English may increase the risk in some ethnic groups performing below clinical cut-off scores, resulting in false diagnoses of cognitive deficit [90].

    RUDAS seems affected by percentage of years in the UK and English proficiency. These results do not replicate the findings reported in the literature [34, 36, 67, 70, 71, 91, 92] which indicated that RUDAS scores were not significantly affected by culturally and linguistically diverse status. This discrepancy between the current and earlier studies may be explained by the use of interpreters in earlier studies [20, 37]. Cognitive scores on the RUDAS between the South Asian and British groups differed from previous studies [71]. The RUDAS was originally designed as a cross-cultural assessment for dementia and scores for both groups were expected to be similar [36]. One potential drawback of the RUDAS is the number of action tasks requiring constant feedback from both the tester and subjects (Praxis and Body Orientation Task) [36]. In fact, whereas CLNT and Mini-ACE only had one significant predictor of group differences, RUDAS had three (i.e., percentage of years in the UK, English proficiency and education level).

    The VSTMBT

    The VSTMBT has proved to be insensitive to the individual’s ethnicity. The VSTMBT indexes low level of visual functions and makes it language-independent, hence a high level of English proficiency is not required [32, 83]. The task relies on a simple set of instructions easy to understand and follow by people with little formal education and assessed in a language they are not proficient [33, 83]. It is also not affected by prior knowledge, experience, or nor practice effects. Previous studies showed that level of education does not affect the performance which indicates that it may be appropriate to test patients from various educational and socio-cultural backgrounds [32, 54], however, none of these earlier studies considered EMs. Here we have further demonstrated that even the most taxing version of this task (i.e., Set Size 3 [83]) did not render it more challenging for EMs. This is important because it was recently suggested that while the version with three items can aid dementia’s risk screening in asymptomatic stages, the version with two items would help detect Alzheimer’s disease dementia in its mild stage [83]. Our results suggest that EMs in the UK can benefit from such an assessment from the asymptomatic stages to the early clinical stages of the disease.

    Interactions between ethnicity and neuropsychological profiles

    Our results suggest that a one-size-fits-all approach would not help identify the vulnerabilities of cognitive and neuropsychological assessment to sociocultural factors. Mungas et al. (2009) [93] suggested that such task properties would not only influence our interpretation of the association between sociocultural factors and behaviours, but also our interpretation of the association between brain pathology and behavioural outcomes. Complex influences seem to drive the sensitivity of traditional neuropsychological tasks to sociocultural as demonstrated by regression models. Different tests showed different vulnerabilities to different sociocultural factors, an observation that is in line with [93]. Taken together these results suggest that assessments for dementia risk in EM groups ought to consider a wide range of meaningful sociocultural factors when it comes to the selection of appropriate tools. For example, linguistic barriers significantly influence the accuracy and validity of the cognitive evaluation, despite the presence of a professional interpreter [94]. Our study is innovative because we investigated the effect of ethnicity on neuropsychological test results when administered in a language other than the participant’s native language without the use of interpreters. This allowed us to observe what can happen in the real clinical testing setting where interpreters are neither always available nor reliable [4, 12, 13].

    Limitations

    Our study had some limitations. The small sample size may have led to some bias [95] considering that South Asians in the UK are not a homogeneous ethnic group, but include Indians 2.3%, Pakistanis 1.9%, Bangladeshis 0.7% and other Asians [57]. Although the South Asians and British were matched for age, education and gender; occupations between the two groups were not perfectly equivalent. In terms of predictor variables, English proficiency was a self-rated measure of language competence. As such, this proxy variable may not necessarily be reflective of the participants’ true level of English literacy in which self-knowledge bias may play a role [96]. EM differ from the majority group more than language. A range of variables are likely to contribute to the difference in test performance, such as acculturation [85], education [97] or socio-economic status [98]. The contributions of the latter variable cannot be fully disentangled by the present study because it was not collected for ethical reasons. Education was measured based on stratification of years of education. However, it is unlikely that quantitative variations in education reflect qualitative differences in education across countries with different education systems [99].

    Abbreviations

    CLNT:

    Cross-Linguistic Naming Test

    EM:

    ethnic minorities

    M-ACE:

    Mini Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination

    MAS:

    Multi-Ethnic Acculturation Scale

    MTL:

    medial temporal lobe

    RUDAS:

    Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale

    VSTMBT:

    Visual Short-Term Memory Binding Test

    Supplementary material

    The supplementary tables for this article are available at: https://www.explorationpub.com/uploads/Article/file/1001227_sup_1.pdf.

    Declarations

    Acknowledgments

    Special thanks to Alfredo Ardila who provided both the Cross-Linguistic Naming Test and Multi-Ethnic Acculturation Scale.

    Author contributions

    CC: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Supervision, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing. MAP: Formal analysis, Writing—review & editing. Both authors read and approved the submitted version.

    Conflicts of interest

    The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

    Ethical approval

    The data collected in this manuscript were obtained in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study received ethical approval by the University of Edinburgh’s School of Health in Social Science (CLIN623).

    Consent to participate

    The informed consent to participate in the study was obtained from all participants.

    Consent to publication

    Not applicable.

    Availability of data and materials

    Datasets are available on request: The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

    Funding

    Not applicable.

    Copyright

    © The Author(s) 2024.

    References

    2020 Alzheimer Europe Policy Briefing: Policy briefing on intercultural care and support for people with dementia and their informal carers/supporters [Internet]. Senningerberg: Alzheimer Europe; c2021–2024 [cited 2023 Sep 9]. Available from: https://www.alzheimer-europe.org/resources/publications/2020-alzheimer-europe-policy-briefing-policy-briefing-intercultural-care-and?language_content_entity=en
    Catney G. Ethnic diversity is increasing: does this mean the UK is becoming more segregated? Geography. 2020;105:348. [DOI]
    World Alzheimer Report 2015 [Internet]. Alzheimer’s Disease International; [cited 2023 Sep 9]. Available from: https://www.alzint.org/resource/world-alzheimer-report-2015
    Sagbakken M, Spilker RS, Nielsen TR. Dementia and immigrant groups: a qualitative study of challenges related to identifying, assessing, and diagnosing dementia. BMC Health Serv Res. 2018;18:910. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Gove D, Nielsen TR, Smits C, Plejert C, Rauf MA, Parveen S, et al. The challenges of achieving timely diagnosis and culturally appropriate care of people with dementia from minority ethnic groups in Europe. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2021;36:18238. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Nielsen TR. Cognitive Assessment in Culturally, Linguistically, and Educationally Diverse Older Populations in Europe. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2022;37:15333175221117006. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Ayalon L, Areán PA. Knowledge of Alzheimer’s disease in four ethnic groups of older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2004;19:517. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Mukadam N, Waugh A, Cooper C, Livingston G. What would encourage help-seeking for memory problems among UK-based South Asians? A qualitative study. BMJ Open. 2015;5:e007990. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Richards M, Brayne C, Dening T, Abas M, Carter J, Price M, et al. Cognitive function in UK community-dwelling African Caribbean and white elders: a pilot study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15:62130. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Fujii DEM. Developing a cultural context for conducting a neuropsychological evaluation with a culturally diverse client: the ECLECTIC framework. Clin Neuropsychol. 2018;32:135692. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Irani F, editor. Cultural Diversity in Neuropsychological Assessment. Developing Understanding through Global Case Studies. New York (NY): Routledge; 2022.
    Franzen S; European Consortium on Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology (ECCroN); Watermeyer TJ, Pomati S, Papma JM, Nielsen TR, Narme P, Mukadam N, et al. Cross-cultural neuropsychological assessment in Europe: Position statement of the European Consortium on Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology (ECCroN). Clin Neuropsychol. 2022;36:54657. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Fernández AL, Evans J, editors. Understanding Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology: Science, Testing, and Challenges. London: Routledge; 2022.
    Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:18998. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Bruno D, Schurmann Vignaga S. Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination III in the diagnosis of dementia: a critical review. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2019;15:4417. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Sheehan B. Assessment scales in dementia. Ther Adv Neurol Disord. 2012;5:34958. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Low LF, Harrison F, Kochan NA, Draper B, Slavin MJ, Reppermund S, et al. Can mild cognitive impairment be accurately diagnosed in english speakers from linguistic minorities? Results from the Sydney Memory and Ageing study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;20:86677. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Carstairs JR, Myors B, Shores EA, Fogarty G. Influence of language background on tests of cognitive abilities: Australian data. Aust Psychol. 2006;41:4854. [DOI]
    Mirza N, Panagioti M, Waheed MW, Waheed W. Reporting of the translation and cultural adaptation procedures of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination version III (ACE-III) and its predecessors: a systematic review. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2017;17:141. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Naqvi RM, Haider S, Tomlinson G, Alibhai S. Cognitive assessments in multicultural populations using the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale: a systematic review and meta-analysis. CMAJ. 2015;187:E16975. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Ardila A. Who Are the Spanish Speakers? An Examination of Their Linguistic, Cultural, and Societal Commonalities and Differences. Hisp J Behav Sci. 2020;42:4161. [DOI]
    Ardila A, Rosselli M, Rosas P. Neuropsychological assessment in illiterates: visuospatial and memory abilities. Brain Cogn. 1989;11:14766. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Rosselli M, Ardila A. The impact of culture and education on non-verbal neuropsychological measurements: a critical review. Brain Cogn. 2003;52:32633. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Waheed W, Mirza N, Waheed MW, Blakemore A, Kenning C, Masood Y, et al. Recruitment and methodological issues in conducting dementia research in British ethnic minorities: A qualitative systematic review. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2020;29:e1806. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Babulal GM, Quiroz YT, Albensi BC, Arenaza-Urquijo E, Astell AJ, Babiloni C, et al.; International Society to Advance Alzheimer’s Research and Treatment, Alzheimer’s Association. Perspectives on ethnic and racial disparities in Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias: Update and areas of immediate need. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15:292312. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Parra MA. Overcoming barriers in cognitive assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Neuropsychol. 2014;8:958. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Parra MA. Barriers to Effective Memory Assessments for Alzheimer's Disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2022;90:9818. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Parra MA, Baez S, Allegri R, Nitrini R, Lopera F, Slachevsky A, et al. Dementia in Latin America: Assessing the present and envisioning the future. Neurology. 2018;90:22231. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Parra MA, Baez S, Sedeño L, Gonzalez Campo C, Santamaría-García H, Aprahamian I, et al. Dementia in Latin America: Paving the way toward a regional action plan. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17:295313. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Parra MA, Butler S, McGeown WJ, Brown Nicholls LA, Robertson DJ. Globalising strategies to meet global challenges: the case of ageing and dementia. J Glob Health. 2019;9:020310. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Franzen S, van den Berg E, Goudsmit M, Jurgens CK, van de Wiel L, Kalkisim Y, et al. A Systematic Review of Neuropsychological Tests for the Assessment of Dementia in Non-Western, Low-Educated or Illiterate Populations. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2020;26:33151. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Della Sala S, Kozlova I, Stamate A, Parra MA. A transcultural cognitive marker of Alzheimer’s Disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2018;33:84956. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Logie RH, Parra MA, Della Sala S. From Cognitive Science to Dementia Assessment. Policy Insights Behav Brain Sci. 2015;2:8191. [DOI]
    Custodio N, Montesinos R, Diaz MM, Herrera-Perez E, Chavez K, Alva-Diaz C, et al. Performance of the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale for the Detection of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in a Diverse Cohort of Illiterate Persons From Rural Communities in Peru. Front Neurol. 2021;12:629325. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Fernández AL, Marcopulos BA. Cross-cultural tests in neuropsychology: A review of recent studies and a modest proposal. In: Koffler S, Mahone EM, Marcopulos BA, Johnson-Greene D, Smith G, editors. Neuropsychology: A review of Science and Practice, volume III. New York (NY): Oxford University Press; 2019. pp. 93–128.
    Storey JE, Rowland JT, Basic D, Conforti DA, Dickson HG. The Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale (RUDAS): a multicultural cognitive assessment scale. Int Psychogeriatr. 2004;16:1331. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Nielsen TR, Jørgensen K. Cross-cultural dementia screening using the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int Psychogeriatr. 2020;32:103144. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Goudsmit M, van Campen J, Schilt T, Hinnen C, Franzen S, Schmand B. One Size Does Not Fit All: Comparative Diagnostic Accuracy of the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale and the Mini Mental State Examination in a Memory Clinic Population with Very Low Education. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2018;8:290305. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Working with interpreters: guidelines for psychologists [Internet]. The British Psychological Society; c2000–2023 [cited 2023 Sep 10]. Available from: https://www.bps.org.uk/guideline/working-interpreters-guidelines-psychologists
    #Registrationrefused: A Report by Doctors of The World UK [Internet]. New York (NY): Doctors of the World; c2024 [cited 2023 Sep 10]. Available from: https://doctorsoftheworld.org/blog/registrationrefused
    Pollard T, Howard N. Mental healthcare for asylum-seekers and refugees residing in the United Kingdom: a scoping review of policies, barriers, and enablers. Int J Ment Health Syst. 2021;15:60. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Interpreting, communication support and translation national policy [Internet]. Public Health Scotland; [cited 2023 Sep 9]. Available from: https://publichealthscotland.scot/publications/interpreting-communication-support-and-translation-national-policy/
    Baker R. Language testing and the assessment of dementia in second language settings: A case study. Lang Test. 1996;13:322. [DOI]
    Haralambous B, Tinney J, LoGiudice D, Lee SM, Lin X. Interpreter-mediated Cognitive Assessments: Who Wins and Who Loses? Clin Gerontol. 2018;41:22736. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Gerrish K, Chau R, Sobowale A, Birks E. Bridging the language barrier: the use of interpreters in primary care nursing. Health Soc Care Community. 2004;12:40713. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Rosenberg E, Seller R, Leanza Y. Through interpreters’ eyes: comparing roles of professional and family interpreters. Patient Educ Couns. 2008;70:8793. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Ardila A, Rosselli M, Puente AE. Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Spanish Speaker. New York (NY): Springer; 1994.
    Artiola i Fortuny L, Mullaney HA. Assessing Patients Whose Language You Do Not Know: Can The Absurd Be Ethical? Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;12:11326. [DOI]
    Puente AE, Perez-Garcia M, Lopez RV, Hidalgo-Ruzzante NA, Fasfous AF. Chapter 12 - Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Educationally Dissimilar Individuals. In: Paniagua FA, Yamada AM, editors. Handbook of Multicultural Mental Health. 2nd ed. Academic Press; 2013. pp. 225–41.
    Melikyan ZA, Agranovich AV, Puente AE. Fairness in psychological testing. In: Goldstein G, Allen DN, DeLuca J, editors. Handbook of Psychological Assessment. 4th ed. Elsevier Academic Press; 2019. pp. 551–72.
    Hsieh S, McGrory S, Leslie F, Dawson K, Ahmed S, Butler CR, et al. The Mini-Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination: a new assessment tool for dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2015;39:111. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Ardila A. Toward the development of a cross-linguistic naming test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;22:297307. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Parra MA, Abrahams S, Logie RH, Della Sala S. Visual short-term memory binding in Alzheimer’s disease and depression. J Neurol. 2010;257:11609. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Parra MA, Abrahams S, Logie RH, Méndez LG, Lopera F, Della Sala S. Visual short-term memory binding deficits in familial Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2010;133:270213. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Ardila A, Maslova OV, Novikova IA, Shlyakhta DA, Aguilar YY. Acculturation Scale to Russia (ASR) for International Students: Development and Psychometric Verification. RUDN J Psychol Pedag. 2019;16:393415. Russian. [DOI]
    Ethnicity [Internet]. Edinburgh: Scotland’s Census; c2021 [cited 2023 Sep 11]. Available from: https://www.scotlandscensus.gov.uk/census-results/at-a-glance/ethnicity/
    South Asians in the United Kingdom [Internet]. Minority Rights Group; [cited 2023 Sep 11]. Available from: https://minorityrights.org/minorities/south-asians/
    Swadesh M. Towards Greater Accuracy in Lexicostatistic Dating. Int J Am Linguist. 1955;21:12137. [DOI]
    Tadmor U. Borrowability and the notion of basic vocabulary. Diachronica. 2010;27:22646. [DOI]
    Gálvez-Lara M, Moriana JA, Vilar-López R, Fasfous AF, Hidalgo-Ruzzante N, Pérez-García M. Validation of the cross-linguistic naming test: a naming test for different cultures? A preliminary study in the Spanish population. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2015;37:10212. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Abou-Mrad F, Chelune G, Zamrini E, Tarabey L, Hayek M, Fadel P. Screening for dementia in Arabic: normative data from an elderly Lebanese sample. Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;31:119. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Mathuranath PS, Cherian JP, Mathew R, George A, Alexander A, Sarma SP. Mini mental state examination and the Addenbrooke's cognitive examination: effect of education and norms for a multicultural population. Neurol India. 2007;55:10610. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Hammad M, Syed Sulaiman S, Aziz N, Sha’aban A, Saeed M, Mohamed Noor D. Validation of the Malay Version of Mini-Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination: A Pilot Study. Value Health. 2018;21:S78. [DOI]
    Charernboon T. Diagnostic accuracy of the Thai version of the Mini-Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination as a mild cognitive impairment and dementia screening test. Psychogeriatrics. 2019;19:3404. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Yang L, Li X, Yin J, Yu N, Liu J, Ye F. A Validation Study of the Chinese Version of the Mini-Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination for Screening Mild Cognitive Impairment and Mild Dementia. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2019;32:20510. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Matías-Guiu JA, Valles-Salgado M, Rognoni T, Hamre-Gil F, Moreno-Ramos T, Matías-Guiu J. Comparative Diagnostic Accuracy of the ACE-III, MIS, MMSE, MoCA, and RUDAS for Screening of Alzheimer Disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2017;43:23746. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Nielsen TR, Andersen BB, Gottrup H, Lützhøft JH, Høgh P, Waldemar G. Validation of the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale for multicultural screening in Danish memory clinics. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2013;36:35462. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Iype T, Ajitha BK, Antony P, Ajeeth NB, Job S, Shaji KS. Usefulness of the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment scale in South India. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006;77:5134. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Cheung G, Clugston A, Croucher M, Malone D, Mau E, Sims A, et al. Performance of three cognitive screening tools in a sample of older New Zealanders. Int Psychogeriatr. 2015;27:9819. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Gonçalves DC, Arnold E, Appadurai K, Byrne GJ. Case finding in dementia: comparative utility of three brief instruments in the memory clinic setting. Int Psychogeriatr. 2011;23:78896. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Nielsen TR, Segers K, Vanderaspoilden V, Beinhoff U, Minthon L, Pissiota A, et al. Validation of a brief Multicultural Cognitive Examination (MCE) for evaluation of dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;34:9829. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Harrison JE. Commentary: Composite cognitive and functional measures for early stage Alzheimer’s disease trials. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2020;12:e12009. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Schneider LS, Goldberg TE. Composite cognitive and functional measures for early stage Alzheimer’s disease trials. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2020;12:e12017. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Parra MA, Calia C, Pattan V, Della Sala S. Memory markers in the continuum of the Alzheimer’s clinical syndrome. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2022;14:142. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Jonin PY, Calia C, Muratot S, Belliard S, Duché Q, Barbeau EJ, et al. Refining understanding of working memory buffers through the construct of binding: Evidence from a single case informs theory and clinical practise. Cortex. 2019;112:3757. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Parra MA, Della Sala S, Logie RH, Morcom AM. Neural correlates of shape-color binding in visual working memory. Neuropsychologia. 2014;52:2736. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Parra MA, Fabi K, Luzzi S, Cubelli R, Hernandez Valdez M, Della Sala S. Relational and conjunctive binding functions dissociate in short-term memory. Neurocase. 2015;21:5666. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Insausti R, Juottonen K, Soininen H, Insausti AM, Partanen K, Vainio P, et al. MR volumetric analysis of the human entorhinal, perirhinal, and temporopolar cortices. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1998;19:65971. [PubMed] [PMC]
    Bastin C, Delhaye E. Targeting the function of the transentorhinal cortex to identify early cognitive markers of Alzheimer’s disease. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2023;23:98696. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Didic M, Barbeau EJ, Felician O, Tramoni E, Guedj E, Poncet M, et al. Which memory system is impaired first in Alzheimer’s disease? J Alzheimers Dis. 2011;27:1122. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Parra MA, Calia C, García AF, Olazarán-Rodríguez J, Hernandez-Tamames JA, Alvarez-Linera J, et al. Refining memory assessment of elderly people with cognitive impairment: Insights from the short-term memory binding test. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2019;83:11420. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Parra MA, Sala SD, Abrahams S, Logie RH, Méndez LG, Lopera F. Specific deficit of colour-colour short-term memory binding in sporadic and familial Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:194352. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Yassuda MS, Carthery-Goulart MT, Cecchini MA, Cassimiro L, Fernandes KD, Baradel RR, et al. Free Recall of Bound Information Held in Short-Term Memory is Unimpaired by Age and Education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2020;35:16575. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Tan YW, Burgess GH, Green RJ. The effects of acculturation on neuropsychological test performance: A systematic literature review. Clin Neuropsychol. 2021;35:54171. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Forno G, Parra MA, Thumala D, Villagra R, Cerda M, Zitko P, et al. The “when” matters: Evidence from memory markers in the clinical continuum of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology. 2023;37:75368. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Campbell L. Historical linguistics: An introduction. 3rd ed. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2013.
    Brown K, editor. Encyclopedia of language and linguistics. 2nd ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2006.
    Ardila A. Cultural values underlying psychometric cognitive testing. Neuropsychol Rev. 2005;15:18595. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Berry J, Poortinga Y, Segall M, Dasen P. Cross-cultural Psychology: Research and Applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1992.
    Mirza N, Panagioti M, Waheed W. Cultural validation of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Version III Urdu for the British Urdu-speaking population: a qualitative assessment using cognitive interviewing. BMJ Open. 2018;8:e021057. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Basic D, Khoo A, Conforti D, Rowland J, Vrantsidis F, Logiudice D, et al. Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale, Mini-Mental State Examination and General Practitioner Assessment of Cognition in a multicultural cohort of community-dwelling older persons with early dementia. Aust Psychol. 2009;44:4053. [DOI]
    Rowland JT, Basic D, Storey JE, Conforti DA. The Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale (RUDAS) and the Folstein MMSE in a multicultural cohort of elderly persons. Int Psychogeriatr. 2006;18:11120. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Mungas D, Reed BR, Farias ST, Decarli C. Age and education effects on relationships of cognitive test scores with brain structure in demographically diverse older persons. Psychol Aging. 2009;24:11628. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Napier AD, Ancarno C, Butler B, Calabrese J, Chater A, Chatterjee H, et al. Culture and health. Lancet. 2014;384:160739. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Francis JJ, Johnston M, Robertson C, Glidewell L, Entwistle V, Eccles MP, et al. What is an adequate sample size? Operationalising data saturation for theory-based interview studies. Psychol Health. 2010;25:122945. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Karpen SC. The Social Psychology of Biased Self-Assessment. Am J Pharm Educ. 2018;82:6299. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Zahodne LB, Watson CW, Seehra S, Martinez MN. Positive Psychosocial Factors and Cognition in Ethnically Diverse Older Adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2018;24:294304. [DOI] [PubMed] [PMC]
    Iliffe S, Manthorpe J. The debate on ethnicity and dementia: from category fallacy to person-centred care? Aging Ment Health. 2004;8:28392. [DOI] [PubMed]
    Storey JD. A direct approach to false discovery rates. J R Stat Soc Ser B (Stat Methodol). 2002;64:47998. [DOI]